Dear Professor van Dijck,

In November 2017, Professor Jeffrey Harvey of the Netherlands Institute of Ecology, a KNAW institute, was the lead author on a paper published in the journal BioScience.

I requested the data behind the paper, and was pointed to the data archive. Unfortunately, the data released was incomplete.

The paper classifies blogs as “yellow” or “blue”. This classification is hardwired into the R code used to analyse the data. The paper only vaguely describes the procedure for classification: The content of the blogs is compared to the reports of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC). Unfortunately, the data released only contain the home page of the blogs investigated, rather than the specific blog posts examined. Some of these blogs consists of hundreds if not thousands of posts. Similarly, the IPCC has published many reports running to thousands of pages, with positions that change over time and chapters that contradict one another. Agreement or disagreement with the IPCC is therefore a meaningless concept, unless chapter and verse are specified.

I have repeatedly asked Professor Harvey and his co-authors to be more explicit, but to no avail. Specifically, I have asked, in vain, for the list of blogposts examined, for the IPCC statements assessed, and for the score per post and statement.

This may appear to be a detail, but it is a key contribution of the paper. Professor Harvey argues that a blog’s position on the IPCC predicts its position on polar bears. However, not knowing how the IPCC position is derived, it cannot be excluded that Harvey fitted the data to match the conclusion.

In the same paper, Professor Harvey and colleagues analyse the contents of a list of published papers. The released data specify the list of papers, but replicating their query to the Web of Science obtains a different list of papers. I have asked Professor Harvey, in vain, to specify the criteria by which certain papers were excluded; and to specify the query through which papers were added.

This point may seem mundane, but it is the other main conclusion of the paper. Professor Harvey claims that blue blogs agree with “the literature”, and yellow blogs do not. Unfortunately, as Professor Harvey will not explain how “the literature” was delineated, the validity of this conclusion is unknown.

As indicated above, I have repeatedly asked Professor Harvey and his co-authors to release the full dataset. They have refused to do, suggesting that they have something to hide.

I have contacted Mr Melle de Vries, the head of information policy at the KNAW. KNAW data policy is clear: All data should be released, and exceptions specified. My experience with Mr de Vries was frustrating. He kept blandly repeating that all data had been released, which is not true as described above and repeatedly explained to Mr de Vries.

I apologize to have to bring this to your attention. KNAW data policy sets a good example: Scientific data should be open to outside scrutiny. Unfortunately, KNAW researchers do not follow this policy, and KNAW officers do not enforce it.

I would be grateful for your intervention in this specific case, and for your efforts to make sure that KNAW data policy is maintained.

I look forward to your reply.


Yours sincerely,



Richard Tol


A detailed commentary on the paper is available here.
1

View comments

  1. Nature Communications published a paper last week that argues that the media pay undue attention to people who do not worry much about climate change. The journal and authors are now in all sorts of trouble because they identified those people, smeared their name, and released personal data. The journal editor confirmed that she did not check whether the paper had IRB approval.**

    UC Merced confirms that the paper had no such approval*** and argues that it did not need such approval because no data were collected on political views or academic qualifications.**** Yet, paper and press release refer to views and qualification, the paper obliquely as "political origins" and "non-scientific experts", the press release explicitly with its "false authority" and "lack [of] scientific training". If no data were collected on political views and academic qualifications, then how do the authors support these remarks?

    Regardless of the above, the paper is bad. At its core, it compares a sample (A) of researchers who are well published and cited in climate research to another sample (B) who are skeptical of climate policy and prominent in the media. They conclude that sample (A) is better published and cited in climate research while sample (B) attracts more media attention.

    It should be trivial to support a trivial hypothesis, but the authors managed to mess it up. Samples (A) and (B) overlapped and people were removed from sample (A). Rob and Ray Bradley were mixed up. Barbara and Harold Betts, realtors praising the climate of California, were seemingly mistaken for Richard Betts, a climate modeller.* Or maybe its Kelly Betts, a photographer for the local newspaper.* Judith Curry may have been mistaken for a dish of Indian origin.* Media attention for Marc Morano was mostly from Morano's blog. (A secondary conclusion in the paper is that non-conventional media pay more attention to people like Morano.) And the paper ignores those who, without academic credentials, argue for climate policy in media, such as Al Gore, Leonardo di Caprio and Greta Thunberg.

    How did this paper get published? The authors are trained as natural scientists and, moonlighting in the social sciences, may not have been aware of the rules that apply to working with human subjects. For two years, the authors worked with human subjects and never paused to wonder about the ethics or consult with a social scientist. You cannot just go around and identify someone with a "lack [of] scientific training" or as a member of a "political movement" -- not if you collected data to prove your point and certainly not, as seems to have happened here, without such data.

    The editors did not stop them either, nor did the referees.

    The referees did not spot the basic flaws in research design and data collection, errors that were very quickly found post publication. The paper's acknowledgements refer to anonymous referees and James Painter, whose own research does not go beyond basic descriptive statistics.

    Over the last few years, we have seen published a number of papers on the science-media interface that are very bad, so bad that the idea should have been killed over the first coffee. Those papers were challenged but never corrected or retracted. Editors therefore now have a pool of referees who do not know the first thing about research ethics or experimental design.

    If the Nature Communications paper stands, Petersen, Vincent and Westerling will be asked to review similar papers in the future.


    *Ruth Dixon argues that all references to the Laguna Beach Independent were removed in an intermediate step not described in the paper.
    ** De Raneiri, personal communication, 20 August 2019.
    *** Eric Kalmin, personal communication, 23 August 2019.
    **** Luanna Putney, personal communication, 28 August 2019.
    2

    View comments

  2. Elsevier Weekblad plaatste een interview met ondergetekende op 23 Maart 2019. Het gesprek ging over van alles en nog wat. Op de titelpagina roep ik dat een "CO2-heffing [op] bedrijven het domste [is] wat je kunt doen". Dat was voor mij een bijzaak, maar de redactie gooit natuurlijk graag olie op het vuur.

    Een CO2-heffing in een lidstaat van de EU op uitstoot dat al onder het Europese systeem van verhandelbare emissierechten (EU ETS) valt, verhoogt de kosten van klimaatbeleid zonder de uitstoot verder te beperken. De grenskosten zijn oneindig hoog dus dit is echt het domste wat je kunt doen. Dit is het zogenaamde waterbedeffect: De emissiedoelstelling is Europa-breed, en meer doen in een land betekent minder doen in een ander land.

    Stephane Alonso van het NRC viel daar over. Milieuactivisten roepen graag dat ze in naam van de wetenschap handelen, maar hebben vaak weinig kaas gegeten van de klimaatmaterie. Alonso bestempelde mijn uitspraak als "grotendeels onwaar". Hij lijkt niet de relevante regelgeving bestudeerd te hebben, hij schijnt de literatuur niet geraadpleegd te hebben, hij heeft mij niet gesproken, en hij lijkt ook niet met de verantwoordelijke ambtenaren in Brussel en Den Haag gesproken te hebben. Hij heeft twee mensen gesproken die wel wat verstand van zaken hebben, maar laat die eigenlijk nauwelijks aan het woord.

    Er is een overschot aan emissierechten in het EU ETS. Op de korte termijn treedt het waterbedeffect dus niet op, maar aangezien uitstootrechten voor altijd geldig zijn, is het waterbedeffect op de lange termijn onverkort geldig -- al kun je stellen dat de verschuiving meer door de tijd dan door de ruimte is. De EU heeft met aanvullend beleid de overschotten teruggedrongen, maar door het waterbedeffect op de korte termijn versterkt wordt -- precies het tegenovergestelde van wat Alonso beweert.

    Het aanvullend beleid van de EU is onafhankelijk van flankerend beleid door de lidstaten, zodat het lange-termijn waterbedeffect gelijk blijft.

    De reactie van het NRC was veelzeggend. Pas na lang aandringen erkenden ze mijn bestaan. Alonso schreef een hautaine email, waarin hij onderstreepte dat Pieter Boot en Sander de Bruyn* toch echt wereldvermaarde experts zijn, en ook zijn eigen expertise benadrukte: Hij is een Latijns-Amerikanist, die direct na zijn studie journalist werd.

    Na herhaaldelijk aandringen heeft het NRC uiteindelijk een brief geplaatst. Mijn aanname was dat er fatsoenlijke en vakkundige mensen bij het NRC werken, maar die mening heb ik toch wel bijgesteld.



    *Sander en ik werkten tegelijkertijd aan onze proefschriften in de economie aan de Vrije Universiteit.
    0

    Add a comment

  3. Research used to be open access. To read a learned paper, you went to a university library. Academics had privileged access because that library was near their offices. Papers have now moved online, often behind a paywall. Search has much improved and faculty (or their research assistants) no longer have to walk to the library. Others are not so lucky. This is wrong in principle, but the proposed solution, Plan S (Coalition 2018, Measey 2018, Thornton 2018, Rabesandratana 2019), is wrongheaded.

    Plan S, supported by research agencies across Europe, demands that all research funded by public grants should be published in open access journals. Anyone should be able to read any paper.

    The vast majority of people outside academia never read a scientific journal. They can be given free access to, say, six papers per publisher per year. Few would take this offer, but the political demand is satisfied: Taxpayers can read the research they paid for. Newspapers and magazines have successfully implemented this freemium model (Kumar 2014).

    The costs of publication need to be covered somehow. Open access therefore means that subscription fees are replaced by submission or publication fees. Submission fees are more lucrative for publishers as most academics overestimate their chances of getting accepted. Publication fees incentivize editors to accept subpar work.

    A shift from payment-for-reading to payment-for-publishing is fine in principle although many universities will struggle with shifting budget from the library to the departments. Poor universities now have limited access to research, but in the future may have to ration publications. Emeriti have library privileges (at zero incremental cost) but universities may be less keen to pay their publication fees.

    Even if all new research is open access, universities will need to pay subscription fees for older journal volumes. Subscription fees are typically paid for collections rather than individual journals. Publishers may extend those contracts to include block waivers for publication fees. If so, Plan S would not change much. Universities will continue to hand over large sums of money to publishers. The general public will still not read my latest paper.

    Plan S does not address the key issue. Publishers have a monopoly on titles. Professors must read their journals to keep up to date and have no choice but to pay the asking price. Academic publishers make excessive profits, employ excessive numbers of people, and pay excessive wages. Innovation is slow. In short, publishers behave like textbook monopolists (Mankiw 2014).

    Open access does not solve this. Researchers must publish in the best journals for promotion, grants, and prizes. Plan S replaces the monopoly of subscription fees by a monopsony of publication or submission fees. The best journals can and will charge excessive fees.

    Monopolies and monopsonies are best controlled by new entrants, but it takes so long to build the reputation of a journal that this is not a realistic option. The next-best alternative is regulation. Road, rail, power grid, and water treatment are all natural monopolies. When in private ownership, regulation is and should be tight (Mankiw 2014). Academic publishers are like utilities. They provide an essential service and face no competition. Publishers should therefore be treated like utilities.

    Utilities are regulated differently in different countries. The simplest solution is price regulation (Laffont and Tirole 1994): free access for the first six papers, and a flat, low fee for the rest. Aggressive price regulation may bankrupt publishers. Regulators may therefore instead put a cap on the return on capital, and perhaps regulate costs as well, so that prices are lowered rather than white elephants build (Liston 1993).

    The details of regulating a monopoly need careful study. But we first need to recognize the reality. Privately owned companies are sponging vast amounts of money from the largely public higher education sector. Plan S takes a principled stand on a minor problem but ignores a bigger one.



    Coalition, S. 2018. Plan S: Making full and immediate Open Access a reality. Science Europe.

    Kumar, V. 2014. Making 'freemium' work. Harvard Business Review (May 2014).

    Laffont, J. J., and J. Tirole. 1994. Access pricing and competition. European Economic Review 38 (9):1673-1710.

    Liston, C. 1993. Price-cap versus rate-of-return regulation. Journal of Regulatory Economics 5 (1):25-48.

    Mankiw, N. G. 2014. Principles of Microeconomics. Nashville: Southwestern College Publishing.

    Measey, J. 2018. Europe's plan S could raise everyone else's publication paywall. Nature 562 (7728):494.

    Rabesandratana, T. 2019. The world debates open-access mandates. Science 363 (6422):11-121.

    Thornton, J. 2018. Transition to immediate open access publishing under Plan S will be smooth, promise backers. British Medical Journal 363:k5019.
    2

    View comments

  4. Dear Professor van Dijck,

    In November 2017, Professor Jeffrey Harvey of the Netherlands Institute of Ecology, a KNAW institute, was the lead author on a paper published in the journal BioScience.

    I requested the data behind the paper, and was pointed to the data archive. Unfortunately, the data released was incomplete.

    The paper classifies blogs as “yellow” or “blue”. This classification is hardwired into the R code used to analyse the data. The paper only vaguely describes the procedure for classification: The content of the blogs is compared to the reports of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC). Unfortunately, the data released only contain the home page of the blogs investigated, rather than the specific blog posts examined. Some of these blogs consists of hundreds if not thousands of posts. Similarly, the IPCC has published many reports running to thousands of pages, with positions that change over time and chapters that contradict one another. Agreement or disagreement with the IPCC is therefore a meaningless concept, unless chapter and verse are specified.

    I have repeatedly asked Professor Harvey and his co-authors to be more explicit, but to no avail. Specifically, I have asked, in vain, for the list of blogposts examined, for the IPCC statements assessed, and for the score per post and statement.

    This may appear to be a detail, but it is a key contribution of the paper. Professor Harvey argues that a blog’s position on the IPCC predicts its position on polar bears. However, not knowing how the IPCC position is derived, it cannot be excluded that Harvey fitted the data to match the conclusion.

    In the same paper, Professor Harvey and colleagues analyse the contents of a list of published papers. The released data specify the list of papers, but replicating their query to the Web of Science obtains a different list of papers. I have asked Professor Harvey, in vain, to specify the criteria by which certain papers were excluded; and to specify the query through which papers were added.

    This point may seem mundane, but it is the other main conclusion of the paper. Professor Harvey claims that blue blogs agree with “the literature”, and yellow blogs do not. Unfortunately, as Professor Harvey will not explain how “the literature” was delineated, the validity of this conclusion is unknown.

    As indicated above, I have repeatedly asked Professor Harvey and his co-authors to release the full dataset. They have refused to do, suggesting that they have something to hide.

    I have contacted Mr Melle de Vries, the head of information policy at the KNAW. KNAW data policy is clear: All data should be released, and exceptions specified. My experience with Mr de Vries was frustrating. He kept blandly repeating that all data had been released, which is not true as described above and repeatedly explained to Mr de Vries.

    I apologize to have to bring this to your attention. KNAW data policy sets a good example: Scientific data should be open to outside scrutiny. Unfortunately, KNAW researchers do not follow this policy, and KNAW officers do not enforce it.

    I would be grateful for your intervention in this specific case, and for your efforts to make sure that KNAW data policy is maintained.

    I look forward to your reply.


    Yours sincerely,



    Richard Tol


    A detailed commentary on the paper is available here.
  5. In their eagerness to discredit a colleague[1] Harvey et al. (2017) got ahead of themselves. The write-up shows signs of haste – typographical errors (“principle component analysis”, “refereces cited”) and nonsensical statements (“95% normal probability”) escaped the attention of the 14 authors, 3 referees and editor – but so does the analysis. The paper does three things: It creates a database, it classifies subjects, and it conducts a principal component analysis. Details have not been shared on the database construction or the classification (Lewandowsky and Bishop 2016), so I focus on the principal component analysis.

    Principal component analysis (PCA) aims to reduce the dimension of a dataset by a linear transformation of its variables into orthogonal components and limiting the attention to those components that are principal in explaining the variation in the variables. Harvey et al. (2017) reduce seven variables to two. One variable denotes citation of Susan Crockford. This is recorded as a binary variable, that is, no distinction is made between mentioning her work in passing, criticizing her work, and uncritically adopting her position. The remaining six variables denote agreement with the following statements:

    1. sea-ice extent is on average declining rapidly in the Arctic;

    2. sea-ice extent is decreasing only marginally, is not decreasing significantly, or is currently recovering in the Arctic;

    3. changes in sea-ice extent in the Arctic are due to natural variability, and it is impossible to predict future conditions;

    4. polar bears are threatened with extinction by present and future anthropogenic global warming;

    5. polar bears are not threatened with extinction by present and future anthropogenic global warming; and

    6. polar bears will adapt to any future changes in Arctic ice extent whether because of anthropogenic global warming or natural variability.

    Agreement is measured on a binary scale, even though nuances are clearly possible. Taking the first statement, “extent” could refer to sea-ice area or volume, both of which vary over space and seasons so that “average” obtains different meanings, while “rapidly” could mean different things to different people. Similar objections can be raised against the other five statements.

    The data released by Harvey et al. (2017) contains only zeroes and ones, suggesting that either agreement was recorded by a single coder or, less plausibly, that all coders agreed on all 6 statements by all 182 subjects.

    Intriguingly, there are four subjects who appear to argue that sea-ice is neither shrinking nor stable or growing. One subject seems to argue that polar bears are both threatened by climate change and not threatened, and another is recorded as arguing that polar bears are neither threatened nor not threatened. This corroborates the above assertion that statements were coded by a single coder.

    Statements 1 and 2, and 4 and 5 are mutually exclusive, and statements 3 and 6 are close to statements 2 and 4, respectively. A PCA is redundant in a case like this. The analysts artificially inflated the dimensionality of the data, before using PCA to reduce it again.

    Figure 2 in Harvey et al. (2017) plays another statistical sleight of hand. The figure shows many observations taking many different positions. Seven binary observations can take at most 128 positions, rather than the 182 suggested in Figure 2. In fact, there are only 19 different positions in the underlying data. The jitter applied by Harvey et al. (2017) suggests that the “majority view papers” all take a slightly different position on sea-ice and polar bears – as you would expect had you not known about the binary coding. Actually, the 86 papers fully agree with each other, and with 27 of the “science-based blogs”. Figure 1 shows a more faithful depiction, with the first and second principal component on the axes and the size of the circles reflecting the number of observations.

    Figure 1. Principal component analysis. The horizontal axis shows the first principal component, the vertical axis the second one. Colours denote the four classes (green = majority view papers; blue = science-based blogs; red = controversial papers; orange = denier blogs). The size of the circle denotes the number of subjects, with the smallest circle representing 1 subject and the largest 86.

    Figure 2 in Harvey et al. (2017) shows that different classes of respondent[2] differ strongly on the first principal component, but that there are no significant difference on the second principal component. Table 1 confirms this.

    Table 1. The average and standard deviation by observation class for the first and second principal component with seven or six variables per observation.

    P
    majority
    science
    controversial
    denier
    N

    86
    45
    6
    45
    PC1
    7
    -1.50
    -1.25
    1.40
    3.92


    (0.16)
    (0.41)
    (1.39)
    (0.91)
    PC1
    6
    -1.40
    -1.33
    1.12
    3.85


    (0.15)
    (0.31)
    (1.38)
    (0.90)
    PC2
    7
    -0.26
    0.51
    1.30
    -0.18


    (0.03)
    (1.04)
    (1.06)
    (1.20)
    PC2
    6
    -0.05
    -0.04
    1.39
    -0.05


    (0.01)
    0.11)
    (0.87)
    (0.97)

    The second principal component largely reflects statement 6, whether polar bears can adapt to future climate change. Polar bears appear to have survived the onset of two interglacials and one ice age (Lindqvist et al. 2010). Unfortunately, 114 of the 182 subjects do not take a position on this. Harvey et al. (2017) replaced these missing observations with zeroes (after standardization). Omitting this column makes the first principal component more important (explained variance rises from 80% to 89%) and the second principal component less important (explained variance falls from 11% to 5%). This does not affect the qualitative results: The first principal component explains the differences between the types of observations. The second principal component does not have discriminatory power. See Table 1.

    Harvey et al. (2017) thus really show that there are people who worry about sea-ice and polar bears, and those who do not and cite Dr Crockford.

    But Harvey et al. (2017) do not just show that there are two camps. They take sides. Unfortunately, they count noses and argue from authority, rather than assess the strength of the evidence. It is well-known that like-minded blogs often copy or paraphrase material from one another. Similarly, academic papers often repeat a salient conclusion from previous research. Counting noses is a poor method.

    The argument from authority is weakened by examining the 92 learned papers. Of the 86 “majority view” papers, 39 were authored by Steven Amstrup, Rascha Nuijten or Ian Stirling, who are among the alii in Harvey et al. (2017). Another 13 were authored by Andrew Derocher, a frequent (n=10) co-author of Amstrup.

    The paper does not specify how these 92 papers were identified, beyond a “broad keyword search” on the “ISI Web of Science”. The Web of Science returns 179 articles for a query on “polar bear” and “sea ice”. No information is given how the larger sample of relevant papers was reduced to the smaller one used by Harvey et al. (2017). Comparing the relative contribution of the ten most prolific authors according to the Web of Science to their relative presence in Harvey’s sample reveals that the latter is not representative of the former (chi^2(9)=17.4, p=0.04). Research by co-authors Amstrup and Stirling is overrepresented in Harvey et al. (2017), and work by Jon Aars and Oystein Wiig underrepresented.[3] The sample used by Harvey et al. (2017) appears to be a sample of convenience, and unrepresentative.

    In sum, Harvey et al. (2017) play a statistical game of smoke and mirrors. They validate their data, collected by an unclear process, by comparing it to data of unknown provenance. They artificially inflate the dimensionality of their data only to reduce that dimensionality using a principal component analysis. They pretend their results are two dimensional where there is only one dimension. They suggest that there are many nuanced positions where there are only a few stark ones – at least, in their data. On a topic as complex as this, there are of course many nuanced positions; the jitter applied conceals the poor quality of Harvey’s data. They show that these is disagreement on the vulnerability of polar bears to climate change, but offer no new evidence who is right or wrong – apart from a fallacious argument from authority, with a "majority view" taken from an unrepresentative sample. Once the substandard statistical application to poor data is removed, what remains is a not-so-veiled attempt at a colleague’s reputation.


    Acknowledgements
    Peter Roessingh and Bart Verheggen gracefully shared data and code. I borrowed freely from comments by Roman Mureika and Shub Niggurath. Marco NN had useful comments on an earlier version.

    References
    Barta, J. L., C. Monroe, S. J. Crockford, and B. M. Kemp. 2014. "Mitochondrial DNA preservation across 3000-year-old northern fur seal ribs is not related to bone density: Implications for forensic investigations." Forensic Science International 239:11-18. doi: 10.1016/j.forsciint.2014.02.029.

    Crockford, S., G. Frederick, and R. Wigen. 1997. "A Humerus Story: Albatross Element Distribution from Two Northwest Coast Sites, North America." International Journal of Osteoarchaeology 7 (4):287-291.

    Crockford, S. J. 1997. "Archeological evidence of large northern bluefin tuna, Thunnus thynnus, in coastal waters of British Columbia and orthern Washington." Fishery Bulletin 95 (1):11-24.

    Crockford, S. J. 2003. "Thyroid rhythm phenotypes and hominid evolution: A new paradigm implicates pulsatile hormone secretion in speciation and adaptation changes." Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology - A Molecular and Integrative Physiology 135 (1):105-129. doi: 10.1016/S1095-6433(02)00259-3.

    Crockford, S. J. 2009. "Evolutionary roots of iodine and thyroid hormones in cellcell signaling." Integrative and Comparative Biology 49 (2):155-166. doi: 10.1093/icb/icp053.

    Crockford, S. J. 2016. "Prehistoric Mountain Goat (Oreamnos americanus) Mother Lode Near Prince Rupert, British Columbia and Implications for the Manufacture of High-Status Ceremonial Goods." Journal of Island and Coastal Archaeology:1-22. doi: 10.1080/15564894.2016.1256357.

    Crockford, S. J., and S. G. Frederick. 2007. "Sea ice expansion in the Bering Sea during the Neoglacial: Evidence from archaeozoology." Holocene 17 (6):699-706. doi: 10.1177/0959683607080507.

    Crockford, S. J., and S. G. Frederick. 2011. "Neoglacial sea ice and life history flexibility in ringed and fur seals." In Human Impacts on Seals, Sea Lions, and Sea Otters: Integrating Archaeology and Ecology in the Northeast Pacific, 65-91.

    Harvey, Jeffrey A., Daphne van den Berg, Jacintha Ellers, Remko Kampen, Thomas W. Crowther, Peter Roessingh, Bart Verheggen, Rascha J. M. Nuijten, Eric Post, Stephan Lewandowsky, Ian Stirling, Meena Balgopal, Steven C. Amstrup, and Michael E. Mann. 2017. "Internet Blogs, Polar Bears, and Climate-Change Denial by Proxy." BioScience:bix133-bix133. doi: 10.1093/biosci/bix133.

    Hatfield, V., K. Bruner, D. West, A. Savinetsky, O. Krylovich, B. Khasanov, D. Vasyukov, Z. Antipushina, M. Okuno, S. Crockford, K. Nicolaysen, B. Mac, L. Persico, P. Izbekov, C. Neal, T. Bartlett, L. Loopesko, and A. Fulton. 2016. "At the foot of the smoking mountains: The 2014 scientific investigations in the Islands of the Four Mountains." Arctic Anthropology 53 (2):141-159. doi: 10.3368/aa.53.2.141.

    Lewandowsky, S., and D. Bishop. 2016. "Research integrity: Don't let transparency damage science." Nature 529 (7587):459-461. doi: 10.1038/529459a.

    Lindqvist, Charlotte, Stephan C. Schuster, Yazhou Sun, Sandra L. Talbot, Ji Qi, Aakrosh Ratan, Lynn P. Tomsho, Lindsay Kasson, Eve Zeyl, Jon Aars, Webb Miller, Ólafur Ingólfsson, Lutz Bachmann, and Øystein Wiig. 2010. "Complete mitochondrial genome of a Pleistocene jawbone unveils the origin of polar bear." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107 (11):5053-5057.

    Martinsson-Wallin, H., and S. J. Crockford. 2001. "Early settlement of Rapa Nui (Easter Island)." Asian Perspectives 40 (2):244-278.

    Ovodov, N. D., S. J. Crockford, Y. V. Kuzmin, T. F. G. Higham, G. W. L. Hodgins, and J. van der Plicht. 2011. "A 33,000-Year-Old incipient dog from the Altai Mountains of Siberia: Evidence of the earliest domestication disrupted by the last Glacial Maximum." PLoS ONE 6 (7). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0022821.

    Tollit, D. J., A. D. Schulze, A. W. Trites, P. F. Olesiuk, S. J. Crockford, T. S. Gelatt, R. R. Ream, and K. M. Miller. 2009. "Development and application of DNA techniques for validating and improving pinniped diet estimates." Ecological Applications 19 (4):889-905. doi: 10.1890/07-1701.1.

    West, D., C. Lefèvre, D. Corbett, and S. Crockford. 2003. "A burial cave in the Western Aleutian Islands, Alaska." Arctic Anthropology 40 (1):70-86.

    Wilson, B. J., S. J. Crockford, J. W. Johnson, R. S. Malhi, and B. M. Kemp. 2011. "Genetic and archaeological evidence for a former breeding population of Aleutian cackling goose (Branta hutchinsii leucopareia) on Adak Island, central Aleutians, Alaska." Canadian Journal of Zoology 89 (8):732-743. doi: 10.1139/z11-027.




    Notes

    [1] Susan Crockford has a decent publication record (Wilson et al. 2011, West et al. 2003, Tollit et al. 2009, Ovodov et al. 2011, Martinsson-Wallin and Crockford 2001, Hatfield et al. 2016, Crockford and Frederick 2011, 2007, Crockford 2016, 2009, 2003, 1997, Crockford, Frederick, and Wigen 1997, Barta et al. 2014).

    [2] Respondent classes were generated by an unknown process. Blogs were classified on “their positions taken relative to those drawn by the IPCC”. The Working Groups of the Intergovernmental Panel have published 15 assessment reports and many special reports, each one hundreds if not thousands of pages long. Some blogs are small, others very large. It is not known which blog posts were examined for the classification. Harvey et al. (2017) performed two cluster analyses that show that their polar bear and sea ice data can be classified in the same manner as their blogs, but as their original classification is of unknown provenance, these validation tests are meaningless. The current author has repeatedly requested the data, but in vain, in direct contravention of the lead author’s employer’s data sharing policy – see https://www.knaw.nl/en/topics/openscience/open-access-and-digital-preservation/open-access/policy. Note that the journal does not have such a policy – see https://www.aibs.org/public-programs/biological_data_initiative.html.

    [3] Scopus returns 216 articles for the same query. Harvey’s sample is closer to the Scopus sample (chi^2(9)=13.2, p=0.15).

    3

    View comments

  6. Charles Mountbatten-Windsor BA, known for being heir to the throne and his barmy views on intensive agriculture and homeopathy, has teamed up with Tony Juniper BSc, an environmental activist, and Dr Emily Shuckburgh, an atmospheric scientist, to write a book, the Ladybird Book of Climate Change. There are 24 1-page chapters plus 24 pages with illustrations. The text is simpler than the typical report on a football match.

    Simplicity is a virtue. Readers who want more thorough-going fare on climate have plenty to choose from. This book, however, does not just omit irrelevant detail. Rather, it tells only one side of the story.

    The book is brief on our limited understanding of the climate system and our inability to project key aspects of future climate change with any confidence, particularly rainfall. Parts of climate science are controversial and contested. Communicators of climate science often pretend otherwise, but the climate debate is polarized nonetheless. Selective omission breeds distrust. And, indeed, the Crown Estate has a lot of money invested in renewable energy.

    The authors argue that all impacts of climate change are negative. Forgotten are the costs of heating homes in winter, and traffic disruptions, and that rather more people die in cold weather than in hot. Forgotten too is that more carbon dioxide in the air makes plants grow faster and more tolerant of drought. While most research finds that the negative impacts dominate the positive impacts, omitting the latter is simply dishonest.

    The book gives short shrift to the human ability to adapt. People thrive in the tropics and near the poles. Humankind made it through the last ice age and the rapid warming that followed with rather primitive technology. We are an adaptable species. The authors note that a city like London can protect itself against sea level rise – although decisions will need to be made a bit more quickly than about the expansion of Heathrow. Poorer cities would not be able to defend itself. This view ignores coastal protection of centuries and millennia past. It also ignores economic development: A country like Bangladesh will soon be richer than the Netherlands when it started building serious dikes. That was in 1850. We have since invented draglines and computers and other things that make dike building so much better.

    Climate policy gets an equally superficial and one-sided treatment. Greenhouse gas emission reduction would be without cost or sacrifice. In alternative fact, climate policy would be good for health, for jobs, for social cohesion, for motherhood, and for apple pie – even if there were no climate change. Unfortunately, the authors do not explain why so many people voluntarily burn fossil fuels, even pay for the privilege.

    The book skims over the difficulties of reaching agreement, both nationally and internationally, on how emissions should be cut and by how much. It assumes that future political leaders will simply obey the orders of current ones.

    In short, the Ladybird Book presents climate change as a simple and well-understood phenomenon, with devastating impacts, and a costless if not beneficial solution. If this were remotely true, the climate problem would have been solved thirty years ago.

    The book steers clear of the moral dilemmas that plague climate policy. Greenhouse gas emission reduction is an investment with an uncertain return in the future. Energy will be more expensive, which especially hurts the poor. The main beneficiaries are people in poor countries, but development aid and trade reform may bring greater benefits for the same investment. These are tough questions.

    The Prince of Wales and co-authors missed an opportunity. Theirs is not a simple but a simple-minded account of climate policy. The uninitiated will walk from this book with a poor, skewed and incomplete understanding. If you were convinced already of the urgency of climate policy, this book will neither teach you anything new nor strengthen your belief. If you were unconvinced, the book is easily dismissed as the uninformed ramblings of a none-too-bright heir to a great fortune. In fact, the book is so easily dismantled that it may win a few over to the – mistaken – belief that climate change is a ploy by the establishment to fleece the common people.
    0

    Add a comment

  7. A journalist asked me about the latest report by the Task Force on Climate-Related Financial Disclosures. Her questions are in blue, my answers in black.

    In my view, the TFCRFD is primarily a vehicle for Mr Bloomberg to stay in the limelight, and for Mr Carney to save his marriage and promote his future political career.

    My main questions are:- How important is it for companies to have data about climate change risks?

    The TFCRFD distinguishes between two risks.

    The risk of climate change is small for the companies involved. The more severe impacts of climate change are decades or more into the future; and these impacts are concentrated among the poor who are barely served by these companies.

    The risk of climate policy are small too, because emission reduction targets will be relaxed if climate policy turns out to be expensive.

    - Would it change the way they do business, in your view?

    No. But there will be photo opportunities.

    - Would suddenly releasing all this data be unproblematic, or might it cause the kind of destabilising effect Mark Carney has explicitly said he hopes to avoid?

    No. Emissions data for companies are already available if you know where to look. Climate change will impact on future, rather than current assets, and projections of future exposure are contingent and uncertain.

    - If countries do not incorporate these guidelines into law, would reporting data on climate impacts still give these companies an advantage?

    The companies involved hope to curry favour with green consumers, or at least avoid boycotts. 

    This is what she wrote.

    0

    Add a comment

  8. Dear Ms Caulfield,

    Yesterday you voted against a motion that would guarantee the right of EU citizens already in the UK to continue to live and work here.

    I am one of a family of four of such EU citizens. My wife builds sewage treatments plants, a vital if often underappreciated service, for Southern Water. I teach economics at the University of Sussex, probably one of the largest exporters in the area. Our alumni quickly find well-paid and secure jobs. Our children attend the local primary school. We pay our taxes. My wife volunteers in the local PTA. I regularly volunteer my expertise in energy and environment to the Houses of Parliament. We spend most of our income in the local economy. Frequent visits by friends and family from abroad support the local tourist industry. We love Sussex and its people. To the dismay of their grandmother, our children speak English in a Southeastern accent.

    I can interpret yesterday’s vote in one of two ways. Either you think it is acceptable to play politics with other people’s lives, or you would like to see us leave this country. Could you kindly explain why you voted as you did?

    Best regards


    Richard

    -----------------

    Dear Richard

    I did not vote against EU citizens staying in the EU. This was an opposition debate that has no bearing on Government business. It would be wrong to quantify, as in the opposition motion yesterday, what movement of people will be allowed under our negotiated settlement which has only just started. That said the PM made it very clear yesterday that all existing EU citizens will be able to stay in the UK and that work is being done to ensure as many EU workers are able to move freely here once we leave.

    The vote yesterday was the SNP playing politics and deliberately undermining our ability to negotiate the best deal for Britain and ensuring we are able to have free movement of people from the EU. My family are also from the EU and so I have a particular interest in ensuring free movement continues.

    I hope that reassures you.

    Maria

    --------------

    Dear Maria,

    No, this does not reassure me at all.

    You argue that the motion reflects your position and the position of your party leader. Yet you voted against it. Please forgive me for finding that rather odd.

    Please also forgive me for taking offense that you intend to treat the rights of my children and my wife, and many others in similar positions, as cards to be negotiated with.

    Best regards

    Richard

    -------------


    It was an opposition day debate that has no legislative bearing but would undermine the position of the Government to negotiate if it had been seen as a fixed position of Parliament.

    If this had been a Gov motion which would have actually changed the laws in this country then I would have voted against it. It was just a debate. Opposition debates never hold more weight than just being a debate and never hold any legislative power.


    Maria 

    -------------

    Dear Maria,

    Thank you for clearing that up.

    To you and your friends in Westminster, this is just politics. To me, this is about the rights and future of my family.

    Best

    Richard

    -------------

    Dear Richard

    I am not a fan of opposition debates as they are just political debates that have no substance in terms of outcome but I appreciate that they send a message to constituents that does not reflect what will be the outcome of our negotiations but does in fact cause unnecessary anxiety and distress.

    I am hoping to go on to the Brexit select committee where we hold the Government to account on this process and I will very much be ensuring that EU residents who are here have the protection and reassurance they need when the repeal bill comes before parliament.

    Best wishes


    Maria

    ----------

    Dear Maria,

    May I point out that opposition is a crucial part of any democracy?

    Best

    Richard
    1

    View comments

  9. The news that the government is considering turning the Nissan plant into a bonded warehouse -- essentially ceding part of Sunderland to France, much like part of Calais is governed from the United Kingdom -- so that Single Market rules continue to apply, reminded me of a more radical but ultimately easier proposal.

    The Brexit vote was primarily about immigration. The Single Market has four Freedoms of Movement, for goods, capital, services and workers. Brexiteers want to end the FoM for workers. The EU says that the four Freedoms are inseparable.

    The EU is wrong. Liechtenstein and Georgia, Moldova & Ukraine* have three of the four Freedoms, Liechtenstein because it does not want its houses to be bought up by rich foreigners, Georgia, Moldova & Ukraine because the EU does not want another influx of workers willing to accept low wages.

    More pertinently, the Crown Dependencies (Guernsey, Isle of Man, Jersey) also have three of the four Freedoms.

    If those parts of the UK that want to leave the EU -- Mercia, Northumbria, East Anglia, Kent, Cornwall, Wessex, Wales -- are turned into Crown Dependencies, they will be free to control immigration. Article 50 does not need to be invoked, and the rest of the UK -- London, Scotland, Northern Ireland, Sussex -- remains in the EU.

    This proposal implies devolution. It is therefore unlikely that any Westminster politician will support this proposal.

    Update (16 Oct 2016):
    There are no border checks between the UK and the current Crown Dependencies, nor should there be for the proposed Crown Dependencies. With three Freedoms and free travel for tourism and business, border checks are not required. However, residency checks will need to be put in place for buying and renting properties, and for labour contracts.

    The Scottish National Party under Nicola Sturgeon have suggested that the powers to negotiate international treaties be devolved to Scotland. Constitutionally, that proposal is at least as far-reaching as the one above.

    *Update (18 Oct 2016)
    Added Georgia and Moldova. Note that the Deep and Comprehensive Free Trade Area applies to selected sectors only. Note that the Association Agreement with Ukraine has yet to enter into force.

    Update (20 Oct 2016)
    London is now considering London-only visas, and giving serious thought to it.

    Update (23 Nov 2016)
    Sign the petition!

    Update (29 Nov 2016)
    MoneyWeek on exemptions to the Four Freedoms.

    Update (5 June 2018)
    I'm not alone, although some want the whole of the UK to be like Jersey.
    1

    View comments

  10. The IPCC has published an erratum for our chapter in the Fifth Assessment Report.

    Four data points were changed. Two relate to a paper by Roberto Roson and Dominique van der Mensbrugghe. Roson's key contribution was to introduce the impact of climate change on labour productivity into the analysis of the total cost of climate change. The concluding section of that paper presents two estimates per scenario: the total impact, and the share of labour productivity in that total. Michael Mastandrea, the co-head of the Technical Support Unit double-checking the numbers in our IPCC chapter, thought that Roson instead presents the impact of labour productivity and its share in the total. Mastrandrea checked his reading with Roson, who confirmed, and the estimates were changed. This is one of the discrepancies between the IPCC chapter and my paper in the Journal of Economic Perspectives (and the forthcoming paper in REEP).

    Later on, Robert Kopp was checking the numbers again, asked Roson for the underlying data, and found that my original reading was correct. Mastandrea and Roson were wrong. JEP was right, IPCC was wrong. The erratum sets the record straight: The correct estimates by Roson are lower than the incorrect ones.

    The other two changes relate to a paper by Robert Mendelsohn, Michael Schlesinger and Larry Williams. Mendelsohn presents his results for population-weighted temperature changes. Everybody else in this literature uses area-weighted temperature changes, and Mendelsohn duly reports those numbers as well. Double-checking our results, Mastandrea insisted that the population-weighted temperatures are used -- these show positive impacts at a lower temperature because the world population is concentrated in the tropics which are projected to warm more slowly than the globe. Violating IPCC procedure, Mastandrea ignored our protests. This is another of the discrepancies between JEP and IPCC. The erratum sets the record straight. The numbers shown for global warming for different studies are comparable to one another. Mendelsohn's estimates show benefits at a greater warming.

    In sum, the Technical Support Unit of IPCC WG2 introduced four errors into the Fifth Assessment Report. All four errors exaggerate the impact of climate change.

    Update (12 Oct 2016): Chris Field, former chair of IPCC WG2, submitted a call for an erratum to the erratum, reverting the changes made to the Mendelsohn estimates. Field's argument is that Mendelsohn's area-weighted temperatures are land-only. There is no dispute there. Field overlooks, however, that Mendelsohn's populated-weighted temperatures are land-only too (as rather few people live in the ocean). Mendelsohn's area-weighted temperatures are therefore less incomparable to other studies than his population-weighted temperatures.

    Update (16 Dec 2016): Two months later, we're still going back and forth. Field continues to dispute our reading of Mendelsohn. We offered to show both estimates in an amendment to the erratum, but Field just wants the erratum gone.

    Update (29 Aug 2017): Unable to find agreement between the co-Coordinating Lead Authors and the former Working Group Chair, the current Chairs appointed a committee of three to adjudicate. They found against Field's interpretation: Both estimates will be shown in an amendment to the erratum. However, in an apparent attempt to save Field's face, there will be a vaguely worded footnote that is likely to cause confusion rather than clarity.
    0

    Add a comment

Blog roll
Blog roll
Translate
Translate
Blog Archive
About Me
About Me
Subscribe
Subscribe
Loading
Dynamic Views theme. Powered by Blogger. Report Abuse.